Elizabeth Loftus

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
Jump to navigation Jump to search
Elizabeth F. Loftus
Elizabeth Loftus-TAM 9-July 2011.JPG
Loftus in 2010
Born
Elizabeth Fishman

October 16, 1944 (1944-10-16) (age 74)
Los Angeles, California, U.S.[1]
ResidenceCalifornia, U.S.
NationalityAmerican
CitizenshipUnited States
Alma materStanford University
University of California, Los Angeles
Known forStudies of human memory and their application to forensic settings
Spouse(s)
Geoffrey Loftus
(m. 1968; div. 1991)
AwardsGrawemeyer Award (2005)
National Academy of Sciences (2004)
Royal Society of Edinburgh (2005)
Scientific career
FieldsPsychology, Cognitive Psychology, Psychology and Law
InstitutionsUniversity of California, Irvine
University of Washington
New School University
National Judicial College, University of Nevada
Harvard University
Georgetown University Law Center
Doctoral advisorPatrick Suppes

Elizabeth F. Loftus (born Elizabeth Fishman, October 16, 1944)[2][3][4] is an American cognitive psychologist and expert on human memory. She has conducted extensive research on the malleability of human memory. Loftus is best known for her ground-breaking work on the misinformation effect and eyewitness memory,[5] and the creation and nature of false memories,[6] including recovered memories of childhood sexual abuse.[7] As well as her prolific work inside the laboratory, Loftus has been heavily involved in applying her research to legal settings; she has consulted or provided expert witness testimony for hundreds of cases.[7][8] Loftus has been recognized throughout the world for her work, receiving numerous awards and honorary degrees. In 2002, Loftus was ranked 58th in the Review of General Psychology's list of the 100 most influential psychological researchers of the 20th century, and was the highest ranked woman on the list.[9]

Biography[edit]

Loftus grew up in Bel Air, California.[10] Her parents were Sidney and Rebecca Fishman; her father was a doctor and her mother a librarian.[10] When Loftus was 14 years old, her mother drowned.[8][10] In 1968, Loftus married fellow psychologist Geoffrey Loftus, divorcing in 1991[8][10] but remaining friends.[11][12] Loftus has no children.[8][10]

Career[edit]

Loftus received her Bachelor of Arts degree in mathematics and psychology with highest honors from the University of California, Los Angeles in 1966. She received her MA in 1967 and Ph.D in 1970 (both in mathematical psychology and both from Stanford University), the only woman in her cohort.[3] Her thesis was entitled "An Analysis of the Structural Variables That Determine Problem-Solving Difficulty on a Computer-Based Teletype."[13] Loftus took her first academic appointment in 1970 at the New School for Social Research in New York City.[3] Her research during her time there focused on the organization of semantic information in longterm memory.[3] However Loftus soon realized she wanted to do research with greater social relevance.[13] Loftus attributes this realization in part to a conversation with an acquaintance to whom she was describing her findings about semantic memory, who wondered at the cost of the research compared to its value.[13]

The misinformation effect[edit]

In 1973, Loftus accepted an assistant professorship at the University of Washington and used the new position to begin a new line of research into how memory works in real-world settings,[3][7][10] beginning the empirical study of eyewitness testimony.[8] One of the first studies she conducted was the reconstruction of automobile destruction study,[10] which was designed to investigate whether eyewitness memory can be altered by information supplied to them after an event.[14] Previous studies had established that memories were not necessarily accurate representations of actual events but were actually constructed using past experiences and other manipulations. The study showed that the way in which questions were worded altered the memories subjects reported.[5][10] Loftus' next step was to investigate whether asking leading questions, or providing misleading information in other forms, might also affect people's memory for the original event.[5] To answer this question, she developed the misinformation effect paradigm, which demonstrated that the memories of eyewitnesses are altered after being exposed to incorrect information about an event – through leading questions or other forms of post-event information; and that memory is highly malleable and open to suggestion.[5][8][11] The misinformation effect became one of the most influential and widely known effects in psychology,[5] and Loftus' early work on the effect generated hundreds of follow-up studies examining factors that improve or worsen the accuracy of memories, and to explore the cognitive mechanisms underlying the effect.[5][8]

Expert testimony[edit]

Loftus has testified and advised courts about the nature of eyewitness memory in various cases.[7][10][11] This direct involvement with the application of her work to the legal system grew from an article Loftus published in 1974 about the relationship between findings from psychological science and the witness testimony in a murder trial she had observed,[3][7][10] in which conflicting witness memory played a key role in the evidence.[10] Lawyers who read the article began to contact Loftus to consult her about their cases, and judges requested educational seminars about eyewitness evidence, so she began her work as an educator of legal practitioners.[3][10] In 1975 Loftus set a legal precedent when she provided Washington State's first expert testimony about eyewitness memory (specifically, on the topic of eyewitness identification).[7][12] She has since testified in over 250 cases and consulted on many more.[10][11]

Notable cases she has been involved in due to her expertise include the McMartin preschool trial, O.J. Simpson, the trials of mass murderers Ted Bundy, Willie Mak, and Angelo Bueno, the Abscam cases, the trial of Oliver North, the trial of the officers accused in the Rodney King beating, the trial of the Menendez brothers, the Bosnian War trials in the Hague, the Oklahoma City bombing case, and litigation involving Michael Jackson, Martha Stewart, Lewis "Scooter" Libby and the Duke University Lacrosse team.[7][8][10][11][15]

The memory wars[edit]

In the early 1990s, the focus of Loftus' work shifted to investigating whether it was possible to implant false memories for entire events that had never taken place. The impetus for this new line of research was a case for which Loftus had been asked to provide expert testimony in 1990.[10][11][13][16] The unique point in this case was that George Franklin stood accused of murder, but the only evidence against him was provided by his daughter, Eileen Franklin-Lipsker, who claimed that she had initially repressed the memory of him raping and murdering her childhood friend, Susan Nason, 20 years earlier, and had only recently recovered it while undergoing therapy.[10][11][16] Loftus gave evidence about the malleability of memory, but had to concede that she did not know of any research about the particular kind of memory Franklin-Lipsker was claiming to have; Franklin was convicted (though in 1996 he was released upon appeal).[10][11][16]

At that time, many others were also making accusations, both in and out of court, based on recovered memories of trauma.[16] Loftus began work to find out whether some of these recovered memories might in fact be false memories, created by the suggestive techniques used by some therapists at the time and encouraged in some self-help books.[10][11][16] Ethically, she could not try to convince research subjects that they had been sexually abused by a relative as a child, so Loftus had to come up with a paradigm that involved childhood trauma without causing harm to subjects. Her student Jim Coan developed the lost in the mall technique. The method involves attempting to implant a false memory of being lost in a shopping mall as a child and testing whether discussing a false event could produce a "memory" of an event that never happened. In her initial study, Loftus found that 25% of subjects came to develop a "memory," also known as a "rich false memory,"[17] for the event which had never actually taken place.[11][16] Extensions and variations of the lost in the mall technique found that an average of one third of experimental subjects could become convinced that they experienced things in childhood that had never really occurred—even highly traumatic, and impossible events.[16] Loftus' work was used to oppose recovered memory evidence provided in court[11] and resulted in stricter requirements for the use of recovered memories being used in trials as well as a greater requirement for corroborating evidence. In addition, some states no longer allowed prosecution based on recovered memory testimony and insurance companies were more reluctant to insure therapists against malpractice suits relating to recovered memories.[8][10][11]

Criticisms and harassment related to research[edit]

Loftus' first study using the lost in the mall technique was criticized by Lynn Crook and Martha Dean based on the ethics of the subject recruitment method used.[18] Also, Kenneth Pope has argued she inappropriately generalized the findings to draw conclusions about false memories and therapeutic techniques.[18][19] These writers purported to identify errors, exaggerations, and omissions in her research. Loftus published a rebuttal to these critics and stated that the criticisms appeared to be based on personal animosity rather than a valid understanding of the research. Regarding the ethics of the creation of the study, Loftus stated that it was primarily a colleague who piloted the study with his daughter, and later revisited the idea as part of an undergraduate class she was teaching. She also emphasized that participants demonstrated no adverse effects upon follow-up and pointed out that the study design and findings had been replicated repeatedly, demonstrating the soundness of the conclusions.[20] In addition to opposition from fellow researchers, Loftus has been insulted by a prosecutor, attacked by an airplane passenger who recognized her,[8][11] received hate mail and death threats,[11][21] and has had to have protection by security guards while giving invited addresses.[8][10] Her invitation to give the keynote address at the New Zealand Psychological Society's conference in 2000 provoked the society's director of scientific affairs, John Read, to resign from his position and for conference attendees to distribute materials critical of Loftus' work.[22] Loftus stated that she "didn't wear her best jacket" to give her address for "fear of flying tomatoes."[22]

After criticizing the theory of recovered memory and testifying about the nature of memory and false allegations of child sexual abuse as part of the day care sex abuse hysteria, Loftus was subject to on-line harassment by conspiracy theorist Diana Napolis, who believed Loftus was engaged in satanic ritual abuse or assisted in covering up these crimes as part of a larger conspiracy.[23]

"Jane Doe" case[edit]

The case that has arguably had the biggest negative impact on Loftus is that of "Jane Doe" (real name Nicole Taus). In 1997, David Corwin and his colleague Erna Olafson published a case study[24] of an apparently bona fide case of an accurate, recovered memory of childhood sexual abuse.[21][25] Skeptical, Loftus and her colleague Melvin Guyer decided to investigate further. Using public records and interviewing people connected to Taus, they uncovered information Corwin had not included in his original article—information that they thought strongly suggested Taus' memory of abuse was false.[10][11][21] While Loftus and Guyer were conducting their investigation, Taus contacted the University of Washington and accused Loftus of breaching her privacy.[10][11][26] In response, the university confiscated Loftus' files and put Loftus under investigation for 21 months, forbidding her to share her findings in the meantime.[10][11][21][26] She was eventually cleared of all wrongdoing by the university, and allowed to publish her findings[27][28] in 2002.[10][11][26]

In 2003, Loftus, the University of Washington and a few others were sued by Taus regarding the 2002 publication.[10][25] The suit initially involved allegations of invasion of privacy, defamation, fraud, and infliction of emotional distress; 21 counts and causes of action in total.[25][29] However, in February, 2007 the Supreme Court of California dismissed all but one count under strategic lawsuit against public participation legislation.[25][29] The single remaining count was Taus' claim that Loftus had misrepresented herself as Corwin's supervisor in interviewing Taus' foster mother. The case was settled in August 2007 when Loftus' insurance company agreed to a nuisance settlement of $7,500 rather than cover the cost of a trial for the one remaining allegation.[25] Taus was ordered to pay the legal bills for all of the defendants, which amounted to $450,578.50.[25] This order was subsequently appealed.[25] Loftus published her own analysis of the case in 2009.[30][31]

Present day[edit]

In 2001, Loftus left the University of Washington and her Seattle home of 29 years to work at the University of California, Irvine where she is a Distinguished Professor of Social Ecology, and a Professor of Law, and of Cognitive Science in the Departments of Psychology and Social Behavior, and Criminology, Law, and Society.[32][33] She is also director of The Center for Psychology and Law[34] and a Fellow of the Center for the Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.[35] Loftus' work since arriving at UCI has looked at the behavioral consequences and potential benefits of false memories, such as the ability of false memories to reduce the desire to eat certain foods.[6][7][10]

Honors and awards[edit]

Awards[edit]

Year Award
1991 Honorary Fellow (and lifetime member) of the British Psychological Society
1994 In Praise of Reason Award from the Committee for Scientific Investigation of Claims of the Paranormal[36][37]
1995 Distinguished Contribution to Forensic Psychology Award from the American Academy of Forensic Sciences
1996 Distinguished Contribution to Basic and Applied Scientific Psychology Award from the American Association of Applied and Preventative Psychology
1997 James McKeen Cattell Fellow Award from the Association for Psychological Science
2001 William James Fellow Award from the Association for Psychological Science[26][38]
2002 Contributions to Sexual Science Award from the Society for the Scientific Study of Sexuality
2002 Quad-L Award from the University of New Mexico
2003 Distinguished Scientific Applications of Psychology Award from the American Psychological Association; delivered award address at 2003 APA's convention.[33]
2003 Elected Fellow of the American Academy of Arts and Sciences[2]
2003 Elected Thorsten Sellin Fellow of the American Academy of Political and Social Sciences
2004 Elected to National Academy of Sciences
2005 Distinguished Member of Psi Chi
2005 Elected Corresponding Fellow to the Royal Society of Edinburgh
2005 Grawemeyer Award in Psychology from the University of Louisville[39]
2005 Lauds & Laurels Faculty Achievement Award from University of California, Irvine
2006 Elected to the American Philosophical Society.
2007 Elected Humanist Laureate by the International Academy of Humanism
2009 Distinguished Contributions to Psychology and Law Award from the American Psychology-Law Society
2010 Howard Crosby Warren Medal from the Society of Experimental Psychologists
2010 Scientific Freedom and Responsibility Award from the American Association for the Advancement of Science[40]
2012 William T. Rossiter Award from the Forensic Mental Health Association of California
2016 Isaac Asimov Award from the American Humanist Association
2016 John Maddox Prize awarded jointly by Nature, the Kohn Foundation, and Sense About Science[41]

In her acceptance speech for the Scientific Freedom and Responsibility Award, Loftus states that the word "freedom" is personally important to her, as when she began speaking out about repressed memory, she never imagined she would become "the target of organized, relentless vitriol and harassment". Loftus feels that today's world for science is a perilous one and if scientists want to preserve their freedoms they need to speak out "against even the most cherished beliefs that reflect unsubstantiated myths".[42]

Honorary doctorates[edit]

Loftus has also received seven honorary degrees in a variety of fields.[43]

Year Institution Honorary degree
1982 Miami University Doctor of Science
1990 Leiden University, Netherlands Doctorate Honoris Causa
1994 John Jay College of Criminal Justice, New York City Doctor of Laws
1998 University of Portsmouth, England Doctor of Science
2005 University of Haifa, Israel Doctor of Philosophy, Honoris Causa
2008 University of Oslo, Norway Doctor Honoris Causa
2015 Goldsmiths, University of London Doctor of Psychology[44][45]

Positions of leadership and affiliations[edit]

Loftus is a Fellow of the Committee for Skeptical Inquiry's Executive Council.[46] She is a member of the Scientific and Professional Advisory Board of the False Memory Syndrome Foundation.[47] She has also been a member of the Society of Experimental Psychologists since 1990.

Loftus has been the president of the American Psychological Society (1998–99), the Western Psychological Association (1984, 2004–05), and the American Psychology-Law Society. She was on the governing board of the Psychonomic Society (1990–1995). She was also on the board of directors for the Institute for the Study of the Trial (1979–81).

Publications[edit]

Significant journal articles[edit]

  • "Reconstructing memory: The incredible eyewitness". Psychology Today. 8: 116–119. 1974.
  • —; Palmer, J.C. (1974). "Reconstruction of automobile destruction: An example of the interaction between language and memory". Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior. 13 (5): 585–589. doi:10.1016/S0022-5371(74)80011-3.
  • "Leading questions and the eyewitness report". Cognitive Psychology. 7 (4): 560–572. 1975. doi:10.1016/0010-0285(75)90023-7.
  • —; Miller, D.G.; Burns, H.J. (1978). "Semantic integration of verbal information into a visual memory". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory. 4: 19–31. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.4.1.19.
  • "The malleability of human memory". American Scientist. 67 (3): 312–320. 1979. Bibcode:1979AmSci..67..312L.
  • "Silence is not golden". American Psychologist. 38 (5): 564–572. 1983. doi:10.1037/0003-066x.38.5.564.
  • Christianson, S.; — (1987). "Memory for traumatic events". Applied Cognitive Psychology. 1 (4): 225–239. doi:10.1002/acp.2350010402.
  • —; Hoffman, H.G. (1989). "Misinformation and memory: The creation of memory". Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 118: 100–104. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.118.1.100.
  • "The glitter of everyday memory...and the gold". American Psychologist. 46 (1): 16–18. January 1991. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.46.1.16. PMID 1996855.
  • "The reality of repressed memories". American Psychologist. 48 (5): 518–537. 1993. doi:10.1037/0003-066x.48.5.518. PMID 8507050.
  • —; Garry, M.; Feldman, J. (1994). "Forgetting sexual trauma". Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 62 (6): 1177–1181. doi:10.1037/0022-006x.62.6.1177.
  • —; Pickrell, J.E. (1995). "The formation of false memories". Psychiatric Annals. 25 (12): 720–725. doi:10.3928/0048-5713-19951201-07.
  • "Remembering dangerously". Skeptical Inquirer. 19: 20–29. 1995.
  • —; Manning, C.; Loftus, E.F.; Sherman, S.J. (1996). "Imagination Inflation: Imagining a childhood event inflates confidence that it occurred". Psychonomic Bulletin and Review. 3 (2): 208–214. doi:10.3758/bf03212420. PMID 24213869.
  • "Illusions of Memory". Proceedings of the American Philosophical Society. 142: 60–73. 1998.
  • "Lost in the mall: Misrepresentations and misunderstandings". Ethics & Behavior. 9 (1): 51–60. 1999. doi:10.1207/s15327019eb0901_4. PMID 11657488.
  • Mazzoni, G.A.L.; —; Kirsch, I. (2001). "Changing beliefs about implausible autobiographical events". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied. 7 (1): 51–59. doi:10.1037/1076-898x.7.1.51.
  • —; Guyer, M. (May–June 2002). "Who abused Jane Doe?: The hazards of the single case history. Part I". Skeptical Inquirer. 26 (3). pp. 24–32.
  • —; Guyer, M. J. (July–August 2002). "Who abused Jane Doe? Part II". Skeptical Inquirer. 26 (4). pp. 37–40, 44.
  • Bernstein, D.M.; Laney, C.; Morris, E.K.; — (2005). "False beliefs about fattening foods can have healthy consequences". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 102 (39): 13724–13731. Bibcode:2005PNAS..10213724B. doi:10.1073/pnas.0504869102. PMC 1236554. PMID 16079200.
  • Berkowitz, S.R.; Laney, C.; Morris, E.K.; Garry, M.; et al. (2008). "Pluto Behaving Badly: False beliefs and their consequences". American Journal of Psychology. 121 (4): 643–660. doi:10.2307/20445490. JSTOR 20445490.

Books[edit]

  • Learning. Mednick, S.A., Pollio, R. H. & Loftus, E.F. (1973). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
  • Human Memory: The Processing of Information. Loftus, G.R. & Loftus, E.F. (1976) Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum Associates.
  • Cognitive Processes. Bourne, L.E., Dominowski, R. L., & Loftus, E.F. (1979). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
  • Eyewitness Testimony. Loftus, E.F. (1979). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. (National Media Award, Distinguished Contribution, 1980). (Reissued with new Preface in 1996).
  • Memory. Loftus, E.F. (1980). Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley. (Reprinted by NY: Ardsley Press 1988).
  • Psychology. Wortman, C.B. & Loftus, E.F. (1981). New York: Random House (Knopf).
  • Essence of Statistics. Loftus, G.R. & Loftus, E.F. (1982). Monterey, CA: Brooks/Cole.
  • Psychology Today Bootzin, R., Loftus, E., & Zajonc, R. (1983). (5th ed.). NY: Random House.
  • Mind at Play. Loftus, G.R. & Loftus, E.F. (1983). New York: Basic Books.
  • Eyewitness Testimony—Psychological perspectives. Wells, G. & Loftus, E.F. (Eds.) (1984). NY: Cambridge University Press.
  • Psychology (2nd ed.) Wortman, C.B. & Loftus, E.F. (1985). NY: Random House (Knopf).
  • Cognitive Processes. Bourne, L.E., Dominowski, R.L., Loftus, E.F., & Healy, A. (1986). Englewood Cliffs: Prentice-Hall.
  • Eyewitness Testimony: Civil and Criminal. Loftus, E.F. & Doyle, J. (1987). NY: Kluwer.
  • Statistics. Loftus, G.R. & Loftus, E.F. (1988). New York: Random House.
  • Psychology (3rd ed.). Wortman, C.B. & Loftus, E.F. (1988). NY: Random House (Knopf).
  • Witness for the Defense; The Accused, the Eyewitness, and the Expert Who Puts Memory on Trial Loftus, E.F. & Ketcham, K. (1991) NY: St. Martin's Press.
  • Psychology (4th ed.) Wortman, C.B. & Loftus, E.F. (1992) NY: McGraw Hill.
  • Eyewitness Testimony – Civil and Criminal. Loftus, E.F. & Doyle, J.M. (1992) Charlottesville, VA: The Michie Co.
  • The Myth of Repressed Memory. Loftus, E.F. & Ketcham, K. (1994) NY: St. Martin's Press.
  • Eyewitness testimony: Civil & Criminal, 3rd edition. Loftus, E.F. & Doyle, J.M. (1997) Charlottesville, Va: Lexis Law Publishing.
  • Psychology (5th edition). Wortman, C.B., Loftus, E.F., & Weaver, C. (1999) NY: McGraw Hill.
  • Eyewitness testimony: Civil & Criminal, 4th edition. Loftus, E.F., Doyle, J.M. & Dysert, J. (2008) Charlottesville, Va: Lexis Law Publishing. (482 pages)

Public appearances[edit]

In August 2000, Loftus was the keynote speaker at the conference of the New Zealand Psychological Society held in Hamilton, New Zealand.[22] In 2004, she tried to give host Alan Alda a false memory on Scientific American Frontiers.[48] Loftus attended and was a speaker at the Beyond Belief symposium in November 2006.[49] She was a keynote speaker at the British Psychological Society Annual Conference in 2011, held in Glasgow on May 4–6.[50]

In June 2013, Loftus presented at the TEDGlobal Conference in Edinburgh, Scotland. As of November 2018, the video recording of this talk has attracted over 4 million views.[51] She was also the keynote speaker at the 2013 Psychonomic Society Annual Meeting, held in Toronto, Canada on November 14–16.[52]

See also[edit]

References[edit]

  1. ^ "Elizabeth Loftus - Psychology History, on the webpage of Muskingum University (archived copy)". Archived from the original on May 20, 2003. Retrieved November 18, 2012.
  2. ^ a b "Book of Members, 1780–2010: Chapter L" (PDF). American Academy of Arts and Sciences. Retrieved 2011-04-28.
  3. ^ a b c d e f g Bower, G. H., (2007). Tracking the birth of a star. In M. Garry & H. Hayne (Eds.), Do Justice and Let the Sky Fall: Elizabeth F. Loftus and Her Contributions to Science, Law, and Academic Freedom (pp. 15–25). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
  4. ^ Loftus, E.; Jacobsen, S.D. (22 April 2013). "Dr. Elizabeth Loftus: Distinguished Professor of Social Ecology, and Professor of Law, and Cognitive Science at the University of California, Irvine". In-Sight (2.A): 24–39.
  5. ^ a b c d e f Zaragoza, M. S., Belli, R., & Payment, K. E., (2007). Misinformation effects and the suggestibility of eyewitness memory. In M. Garry & H. Hayne (Eds.), Do Justice and Let the Sky Fall: Elizabeth F. Loftus and Her Contributions to Science, Law, and Academic Freedom (pp. 35–63). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
  6. ^ a b Bernstein, D. M.; Laney, C.; Morris, E. K.; Loftus, E. F. (2005). "Inaugural Article: False beliefs about fattening foods can have healthy consequences". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 102 (39): 13724–13731. Bibcode:2005PNAS..10213724B. doi:10.1073/pnas.0504869102. PMC 1236554. PMID 16079200.
  7. ^ a b c d e f g h Zagorski, N. (2005). "Profile of Elizabeth F. Loftus". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 102 (39): 13721–13723. Bibcode:2005PNAS..10213721Z. doi:10.1073/pnas.0506223102. PMC 1236565. PMID 16172386.
  8. ^ a b c d e f g h i j k Neimark, Jill (1996). "The diva of disclosure, memory researcher Elizabeth Loftus". Psychology Today. 29 (1): 48–53. Retrieved 2012-06-12.
  9. ^ Haggbloom, SJ; Warnick R; Warnick JE; Jones VK; Yarbrough GL; Russell TM; Borecky CM; McGahhey R; Powell JL; Beavers J; Monte E (2002). "The 100 most eminent psychologists of the 20th century". Rev. Gen. Psychol. 6 (2): 139–152. doi:10.1037/1089-2680.6.2.139.
  10. ^ a b c d e f g h i j k l m n o p q r s t u v w x y z Saletan, William (2010-06-04). "The memory doctor: the future of false memories". Slate. Retrieved 2012-05-08.
  11. ^ a b c d e f g h i j k l m n o p q r Wilson, A (2002-11-03). "War & remembrance: Controversy is a constant for memory researcher Elizabeth Loftus, newly installed at UCI". The Orange County Register. Retrieved 2009-01-19.
  12. ^ a b Loftus G (2007). "Elizabeth F. Loftus: The early years". In Garry M & Hayne H. Do Justice and Let the Sky Fall: Elizabeth F. Loftus and Her Contributions to Science, Law, and Academic Freedom. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp. 27–31. ISBN 978-0805852325.CS1 maint: Uses editors parameter (link)
  13. ^ a b c d Loftus, EF (2007). "Memory distortions: Problems solved and unsolved". In Garry M & Hayne H. Do Justice and Let the Sky Fall: Elizabeth F. Loftus and Her Contributions to Science, Law, and Academic Freedom. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp. 1–14. ISBN 978-0805852325.CS1 maint: Uses editors parameter (link)
  14. ^ Loftus, EF; Palmer JC (1974). "Reconstruction of Automobile Destruction : An Example of the Interaction Between Language and Memory" (PDF). Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior. 13 (5): 585–9. doi:10.1016/S0022-5371(74)80011-3.
  15. ^ "Memory Expert Pressed In C.I.A. Leak Case". The New York Times. Associated Press. 2006-10-27. Retrieved 2012-06-12.
  16. ^ a b c d e f g Strange, D; Clifasefi S; Garry M (2007). "False memories.". In Garry M & Hayne H. Do Justice and Let the Sky Fall: Elizabeth F. Loftus and Her Contributions to Science, Law, and Academic Freedom. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp. 137–68. ISBN 978-0805852325.CS1 maint: Uses editors parameter (link)
  17. ^ French, Chris (2017). "The John Maddox Prize Nomination for Elizabeth Loftus". Skeptical Inquirer. 41 (2): 20–23.
  18. ^ a b Crook, L.; Dean, Martha (1999). "Lost in a Shopping Mall – A Breach of Professional Ethics". Ethics & Behavior. 9 (1): 39–50. doi:10.1207/s15327019eb0901_3. PMID 11657487. Retrieved 2008-01-18.
  19. ^ Pope, K. (1996). "Memory, Abuse, and Science: Questioning Claims about the False Memory Syndrome Epidemic". American Psychologist. 51 (9): 957–974. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.51.9.957. PMID 8819364. Retrieved 2008-01-31.
  20. ^ Loftus, E (1999). "Lost in the mall: Misrepresentations and misunderstandings" (PDF). Ethics & Behavior. 9 (1): 51–60. doi:10.1207/s15327019eb0901_4. PMID 11657488. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2010-06-19. Retrieved 2010-02-23.
  21. ^ a b c d Abramsky, S (2004-08-19). "Memory and Manipulation: The trials of Elizabeth Loftus, defender of the wrongly accused". LA Weekly. Retrieved 2012-06-10.
  22. ^ a b c Taylor, Annette (1 November 2000). "A Good Time Was Had By All". NZ Skeptics. Archived from the original on 6 March 2016. Retrieved 11 November 2016.
  23. ^ Bocij, Paul (2004). Cyberstalking: harassment in the Internet age and how to protect your family. New York: Praeger Publishers. pp. 34. ISBN 978-0-275-98118-1.
  24. ^ Corwin, D.; Olafson E. (1997). "Videotaped Discovery of a Reportedly Unrecallable Memory of Child Sexual Abuse: Comparison with a Childhood Interview Videotaped 11 Years Before". Child Maltreatment. 2 (2): 91–112. doi:10.1177/1077559597002002001. Retrieved 2008-01-09.
  25. ^ a b c d e f g Tavris, Carol (2008). "Whatever Happened to 'Jane Doe'?". Skeptical Inquirer. 32 (1): 28–30. ISSN 0194-6730. Retrieved 2010-02-23.
  26. ^ a b c d Tavris, C (2002). "The high cost of skepticism". Skeptical Inquirer. 26 (4): 41–44.
  27. ^ Loftus & Guyer 2002a.
  28. ^ Loftus & Guyer 2002b.
  29. ^ a b Taus v. Loftus, 683 3d 54, 775 (Cal. 4th 2007).
  30. ^ Loftus, Elizabeth (May 2008). "Perils of Provocative Scholarship". Observer. 21 (5): 13–15. ISSN 1050-4672.
  31. ^ Loftus, E; Geis G (2009). "Taus v. Loftus: Determining the Legal Ground Rules for Scholarly Inquiry". Journal of Forensic Psychology Practice. 9 (2): 147–62. doi:10.1080/15228930802575524.
  32. ^ "Elizabeth F. Loftus faculty page". University of California, Irvine. n.d. Retrieved 2012-06-12.
  33. ^ a b No Authorship Indicated (2003). "Award for Distinguished Scientific Psychology" (PDF). American Psychologist. 58 (11): 864–73. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.58.11.864. PMID 14609373.
  34. ^ "Center for Psychology and Law Faculty". University of California, Irvine. n.d. Archived from the original on 2012-01-28. Retrieved 2012-06-12.
  35. ^ "CNLM Fellows at UC Irvine". University of California, Irvine. n.d. Archived from the original on 2012-11-07. Retrieved 2012-06-12.
  36. ^ Karr, Barry (1994). "Five Honored with CSICOP Awards". Skeptical Inquirer. 18 (5): 461–462.
  37. ^ Frazier, Kendrick (1995). "Editor's Note: Three Culture Clashes". Skeptical Inquirer. 19 (2): 2.
  38. ^ "William James Fellow Award: Elizabeth Loftus, University of California, Irvine". Association for Psychological Science. Retrieved 2009-01-20.
  39. ^ "2005- Elizabeth Loftus". Archived from the original on 2015-05-18.
  40. ^ "False Memory Investigator Elizabeth Loftus Receives 2010 \Scientific Freedom and Responsibility Award". AAAS. Retrieved 2011-02-20.
  41. ^ Editor, Ian Sample Science (2016-11-17). "'We can't let the bullies win': Elizabeth Loftus awarded 2016 John Maddox Prize". The Guardian. Guardian News. Retrieved 2016-11-17.
  42. ^ Loftus, Elizabeth (May–June 2011). "We Live in Perilous Times for Science". Skeptical Inquirer. 35 (3): 13.
  43. ^ "E. Loftus' Curriculum Vitae". University of California – Irvine. Retrieved 2011-07-02.[permanent dead link]
  44. ^ Loftus, Elizabeth. "Illusions of Memory". Skeptical Inquirer. 40 (1): 22–23.
  45. ^ "Honorary Degree for top psychologist Elizabeth Loftus". University of London. Goldsmiths. Retrieved 12 December 2015.
  46. ^ "CSI Adds to Executive Council". Skeptical Inquirer. May 5, 2011. Retrieved 2011-06-13.
  47. ^ "The FMSF Scientific and Professional Advisory Board – Profiles". Archived from the original on 2012-05-12. Retrieved 2009-01-19.
  48. ^ "Elizabeth Loftus, on season 14, episode 2". Scientific American Frontiers. Chedd-Angier Production Company. 2004. PBS. Archived from the original on 2006.
  49. ^ "TSN: Beyond Belief: Science, Reason, Religion & Survival". Thesciencenetwork.org. Retrieved 2012-04-14.
  50. ^ Loftus, E. (May 2011). "Abstract Details: Conference Proceedings: 2011 BPS Annual Conference". British Psychological Society (BPS). Manufacturing Memories. Archived from the original on 2013-12-02. Retrieved 2013-11-22.
  51. ^ "How reliable is your memory?". TED. June 2013. Retrieved 27 November 2018.
  52. ^ "54th Annual Meeting" (PDF). Abstracts of the Psychonomic Society. 18: cover. November 2013. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2013-12-03.
Sources

Further reading[edit]