Split ergativity
Linguistic typology |
---|
Morphological |
Morphosyntactic |
Word order |
Lexicon |
Split ergativity is a term used by comparative linguists to refer to languages where some constructions use ergative syntax and morphology, but other constructions show another pattern, usually nominative-accusative. The conditions in which ergative constructions are used varies from language to language.
Contents
Nominative-accusative vs. ergative-absolutive[edit]
Nominative-accusative languages (including European languages, with the notable exception of Basque) treat both the actor in a clause with a transitive verb and the experiencer in a clause with an intransitive verb in the same way grammatically. If the language uses case markers, they take the same case. If it uses word order, it is parallel.
For example, consider these two English sentences
Jane was chasing John.
Jane was sweating.
The grammatical role of "Jane" is identical. In both cases, "Jane" is the subject.
In ergative-absolutive languages (including Basque language, Georgian language and Mayan language), there is a different pattern. The patient (or target) of a transitive verb and the experiencer of an intransitive verb are treated the same grammatically. If the two sentences above were expressed in an ergative language, "John" in the former and "Jane" in the latter would be parallel grammatically. Also, a different form (the ergative) would be used for "Jane" in the first sentence. (There is no easy way to represent that construction in English.)
In split ergative languages, some constructions pattern with nominative-accusative, and others with ergative-absolutive.
Split conditions[edit]
The split is usually conditioned by one of the following:
- The presence of a discourse participant (a first or second person) in the proposition. The Australian language Dyirbal behaves ergatively in all morphosyntactic contexts unless one of those is involved. When a first- or second-person pronoun appears, however, it is marked according to a nominative–accusative pattern (with the least-marked case, when it is the agent or intransitive, or with the most marked case, when it is the patient). That can be explained in terms of the high animacy of a first- or second-person speaker in the animacy hierarchy.
- The use of certain tenses and/or aspects in the verb. The Indo-Iranian family, for example, shows a split between the perfective and the imperfective aspect. A verb in the perfective aspect causes its arguments to be marked by an ergative pattern, and the imperfective aspect triggers accusative marking. (Both related languages and others tend to associate the past tense and/or perfect aspect with ergativity.)
- The type of marking involved. Some languages (including various Austronesian languages of New Guinea such as Sinaugoro) have an ergative–absolutive pattern with respect to the marking of case marking but a nominative–accusative pattern with respect to agreement.
- The agentivity of the intransitive subject. In languages like Dakota, arguments of active verbs, such as to run, are marked like transitive agents, as in accusative languages, but arguments of inactive verbs, such as to stand are marked like transitive objects, as in ergative languages. Languages with such a marking are known as split-S languages and are formally a subtype of active languages.
- Pragmatic considerations or for emphasis, contrast, or clarity. In certain Tibeto-Burmese languages, elicited data has consistent ergative, aspectually split-ergative or active-stative case marking pattern, and in natural discourse the “ergative” marking is found only in some clauses, often a minority, usually with some pragmatic sense of emphasis or contrast (DeLancey, 2011).[1]
Examples[edit]
An example of split ergativity, conditioned by tense and aspect, is found in Hindustani (Hindi/Urdu), which has an ergative case on subjects in the perfective aspect for transitive verbs in the active voice. However, in all other aspects (habitual, progressive), subjects appear in the direct case:
लड़का किताब ख़रीदता है laṛkā kitāb xarīdtā hai boy-Msg.DIR book.Fsg.DIR buy-IMPERFECTIVE-Msg be.PRES.3sg[2]
लड़के-ने किताब ख़रीदी laṛke-ne kitāb xarīdī boy.Msg-ERG book.Fsg.DIR buy-PERFECTIVE.Fsg[2]
- "The boy bought a book."
In the first sentence, the verb in present tense has the masculine ending -ā, agreeing with laṛkā (boy), but in the second sentence, the verb in past tense has the feminine ending -ī, agreeing with the feminine noun kitāb (book). "Boy" now appears as laṛke-ne, literally "by the boy".
In Columbia River Sahaptin, the split is determined by the person of both subject and object. The ergative suffix -nɨm occurs only for third-person subjects for which the direct object is in the first or the second person.
ku=š i-q̓ínu-šan-a ína wínš-nɨm and=1SINGULAR 3NOM-see-IMPERFECTIVE-PST me man-ERGATIVE
- "And the man saw me."
ku=nam i-q̓ínu-šan-a imaná wínš-nɨm and=2SINGULAR 3NOM-see-IMPERFECTIVE-PST you.ACCUSATIVE man-ERGATIVE
- "And the man saw you."
ku i-q̓ínu-šan-a paanáy wínš and 3NOM-see-IMPERFECTIVE-PST him/her/it man
- "And the man saw him."
Another ergative suffix, -in, marks the subject in the inverse. Both subject and object are then always in the third-person.
Direct (same as above example):
ku i-q̓ínu-šan-a paanáy wínš and 3NOM-see-IMPERFECTIVE-PST him/her/it man-ERGATIVE
- "And the man saw him."
Inverse:
ku pá-q̓inu-šan-a paanáy wínš-in and INVERSE-see-IMPERFECTIVE-PST him/her/it man
- "And the man saw him."
Notes[edit]
- ^ https://www.academia.edu/3887136/_Optional_ergativity_in_Tibeto-Burman_languages
- ^ a b The morph-by-morph analysis has been simplified to show the features relevant to the topic of split ergativity.
Bibliography[edit]
- Dixon, R. M. W. (1994). Ergativity. Cambridge University Press. ISBN 0-521-44898-0.